Fusiform face area: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m →‎Function: task, replaced: journal=[[Neuroreport → journal=[[NeuroReport using AWB
 
(46 intermediate revisions by 32 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Part of the human visual system that is specialized for facial recognition}}
{{Infobox anatomy
{{Infobox anatomy
|Name = Fusiform face area
|Name = Fusiform face area
|Image = Constudproc - inferior view.png
|Image = Constudproc - inferior view.png
|Caption = Human brain, bottom view. Fusiform face area shown in bright blue.
|Caption = Human brain, bottom view. Fusiform face area shown in bright blue.
|Image2 = Face recognition.jpg
|Image2 = Fusiform face area face recognition.jpg
|Caption2 = Computer-enhanced [[fMRI]] scan of a person who has been asked to look at faces. The image shows increased blood flow in cerebral cortex that recognizes faces (FFA).
|Caption2 = Computer-enhanced [[fMRI]] scan of a person who has been asked to look at faces. The image shows increased blood flow in cerebral cortex that recognizes faces (FFA).
}}
}}

The '''fusiform face area''' - '''FFA''' (''meaning'': spindular/spindle-shaped face area) is a part of the human [[visual system]] that is specialized for [[Face perception|facial recognition]].<ref name="Kanwisher">{{cite journal|date=Jun 1, 1997|title=The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception|journal=J. Neurosci.|volume=17|issue=11|pages=4302–11|pmid=9151747|vauthors=Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM}}<!--| accessdate= 2007-12-04--></ref> It is located in the [[Inferior temporal gyrus|Inferior temporal cortex (IT)]], in the [[fusiform gyrus]] ([[Brodmann area 37]]).
The '''fusiform face area''' ('''FFA''', meaning [[fusiform|spindle-shaped]] face area) is a part of the human [[visual system]] (while also activated in people blind from birth<ref name="blind_from_birth" />) that is [[Functional specialization (brain)|specialized]] for [[face perception|facial recognition]].<ref name="Kanwisher">{{cite journal|author-link=Nancy Kanwisher|vauthors=Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM|date=Jun 1, 1997|title=The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception|journal=J. Neurosci.|volume=17|issue=11|pages=4302–11|doi=10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997|pmc=6573547|pmid=9151747|doi-access=free}}<!--| access-date= 2007-12-04--></ref> It is located in the [[Inferior temporal gyrus|inferior temporal cortex (IT)]], in the [[fusiform gyrus]] ([[Brodmann area 37]]).


==Structure==
==Structure==
Line 14: Line 16:


==Function==
==Function==
The human FFA was first described by [[Justine Sergent]] in 1992<ref name="Sergent">{{cite journal|date=Feb 1992|title=Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. A positron emission tomography study|journal=Brain|volume=115|issue=1|pages=15–36|doi=10.1093/brain/115.1.15|pmid=1559150|vauthors=Sergent J, Ohta S, MacDonald B}}<!--| accessdate= 2007-12-04 --></ref> and later named by [[Nancy Kanwisher]] in 1997<ref name="Kanwisher">{{cite journal|date=Jun 1, 1997|title=The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception|journal=J. Neurosci.|volume=17|issue=11|pages=4302–11|pmid=9151747|vauthors=Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM}}<!--| accessdate= 2007-12-04--></ref> who proposed that the existence of the FFA is evidence for [[domain specificity]] in the visual system. Studies have recently shown that the FFA is composed of functional clusters that are at a finer spatial scale than prior investigations have measured.<ref name="Weiner">{{cite journal|date=Oct 2010|title=Sparsely-distributed organization of face and limb activations in human ventral temporal cortex|journal=NeuroImage|volume=52|issue=4|pages=1559–73|doi=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.262|pmid=20457261|vauthors=Weiner KS, Grill-Spector K|pmc=3122128}}<!--| accessdate= 2010-05-10 --></ref> Electrical stimulation of these functional clusters selectively distorts face perception, which is causal support for the role of these functional clusters in perceiving the facial image.<ref name="Parvizi">{{cite journal|date=Oct 2012|title=Electrical stimulation of human fusiform face-selective regions distorts face perception|journal=J Neurosci|volume=32|issue=43|pages=14915–20|doi=10.1523/jneurosci.2609-12.2012|pmid=23100414|vauthors=Parvizi J, Jacques C, Foster BL, Witthoft N, Rangarajan V, Weiner KS, Grill-Spector K|pmc=3517886}}<!--| accessdate= 2012-10-24 --></ref> While it is generally agreed that the FFA responds more to faces than to most other categories, there is debate about whether the FFA is uniquely dedicated to face processing, as proposed by Nancy Kanwisher and others, or whether it participates in the processing of other objects. The expertise hypothesis, as championed by [[Isabel Gauthier]] and others, offers an explanation for how the FFA becomes selective for faces in most people. The expertise hypothesis suggests that the FFA is a critical part of a network that is important for individuating objects that are visually similar because they share a common configuration of parts. Gauthier et al., in an adversarial collaboration with Kanwisher,<ref>{{Cite arxiv|last=Gauthier|first=Isabel|date=2017-02-22|title=The Quest for the FFA led to the Expertise Account of its Specialization|eprint=1702.07038}}</ref> tested both car and bird experts, and found some activation in the FFA when car experts were identifying cars and when bird experts were identifying birds.<ref name="Gauthier">{{cite journal|date=Feb 2000|title=Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition|journal=Nat. Neurosci.|volume=3|issue=2|pages=191–7|doi=10.1038/72140|pmid=10649576|vauthors=Gauthier I, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Anderson AW}}<!--| accessdate= 2007-12-04 --></ref> This finding has been replicated,<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Xu|first=Y.|date=2005-08-01|title=Revisiting the Role of the Fusiform Face Area in Visual Expertise|url=https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article-lookup/doi/10.1093/cercor/bhi006|journal=Cerebral Cortex|language=en|volume=15|issue=8|pages=1234–1242|doi=10.1093/cercor/bhi006|issn=1047-3211|pmid=15677350}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last=McGugin|first=Rankin Williams|last2=Gatenby|first2=J. Christopher|last3=Gore|first3=John C.|last4=Gauthier|first4=Isabel|date=2012-10-16|title=High-resolution imaging of expertise reveals reliable object selectivity in the fusiform face area related to perceptual performance|url=http://www.pnas.org/content/109/42/17063|journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences|language=en|volume=109|issue=42|pages=17063–17068|doi=10.1073/pnas.1116333109|issn=0027-8424|pmc=3479484|pmid=23027970|bibcode=2012PNAS..10917063M}}</ref> and expertise effects in the FFA have been found for other categories such as chess displays<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Bilalić|first=Merim|last2=Langner|first2=Robert|last3=Ulrich|first3=Rolf|last4=Grodd|first4=Wolfgang|date=2011-07-13|title=Many Faces of Expertise: Fusiform Face Area in Chess Experts and Novices|url=http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/doi/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5727-10.2011|journal=Journal of Neuroscience|volume=31|issue=28|pages=10206–10214|doi=10.1523/jneurosci.5727-10.2011|pmid=21752997}}</ref> and x-rays.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Bilalić|first=Merim|last2=Grottenthaler|first2=Thomas|last3=Nägele|first3=Thomas|last4=Lindig|first4=Tobias|date=2016-03-01|title=The Faces in Radiological Images: Fusiform Face Area Supports Radiological Expertise|url=https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article-lookup/doi/10.1093/cercor/bhu272|journal=Cerebral Cortex|language=en|volume=26|issue=3|pages=1004–1014|doi=10.1093/cercor/bhu272|issn=1047-3211}}</ref> Recently, it was found that the thickness of the cortex in the FFA predicts the ability to recognize faces as well as vehicles.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=McGugin|first=Rankin W.|last2=Van Gulick|first2=Ana E.|last3=Gauthier|first3=Isabel|date=2015-10-06|title=Cortical Thickness in Fusiform Face Area Predicts Face and Object Recognition Performance|url=http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/10.1162/jocn_a_00891|journal=Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience|volume=28|issue=2|pages=282–294|doi=10.1162/jocn_a_00891|issn=0898-929X|pmc=5034353|pmid=26439272}}</ref>
The human FFA was first described by [[Justine Sergent]] in 1992<ref name="Sergent">{{cite journal|vauthors=Sergent J, Ohta S, MacDonald B|date=Feb 1992|title=Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. A positron emission tomography study|journal=Brain|volume=115|issue=1|pages=15–36|doi=10.1093/brain/115.1.15|pmid=1559150}}<!--| access-date= 2007-12-04 --></ref> and later named by [[Nancy Kanwisher]] in 1997<ref name="Kanwisher"/> who proposed that the existence of the FFA is evidence for [[domain specificity]] in the visual system. Studies have recently shown that the FFA is composed of functional clusters that are at a finer spatial scale than prior investigations have measured.<ref name="Weiner">{{cite journal|last1=Weiner|first1=Kevin S.|last2=Grill-Spector|first2=Kalanit|author-link2=Kalanit Grill-Spector|date=Oct 2010|title=Sparsely-distributed organization of face and limb activations in human ventral temporal cortex|journal=NeuroImage|volume=52|issue=4|pages=1559–73|doi=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.262|pmc=3122128|pmid=20457261}}<!--| access-date= 2010-05-10 --></ref> Electrical stimulation of these functional clusters selectively distorts face perception, which is causal support for the role of these functional clusters in perceiving the facial image.<ref name="Parvizi">{{cite journal|vauthors=Parvizi J, Jacques C, Foster BL, Witthoft N, Rangarajan V, Weiner KS, Grill-Spector K|date=Oct 2012|title=Electrical stimulation of human fusiform face-selective regions distorts face perception|journal=J Neurosci|volume=32|issue=43|pages=14915–20|doi=10.1523/jneurosci.2609-12.2012|pmc=3517886|pmid=23100414}}<!--| access-date= 2012-10-24 --></ref> While it is generally agreed that the FFA responds more to faces than to most other categories, there is debate about whether the FFA is uniquely dedicated to face processing, as proposed by Nancy Kanwisher and others, or whether it participates in the processing of other objects. The expertise hypothesis, as championed by [[Isabel Gauthier]] and others, offers an explanation for how the FFA becomes selective for faces in most people. The expertise hypothesis suggests that the FFA is a critical part of a network that is important for individuating objects that are visually similar because they share a common configuration of parts. Gauthier et al., in an adversarial collaboration with Kanwisher,<ref>{{Cite arXiv|last=Gauthier|first=Isabel|author-link=Isabel Gauthier|date=2017-02-22|title=The Quest for the FFA led to the Expertise Account of its Specialization|eprint=1702.07038|class=q-bio.NC}}</ref> tested both car and bird experts, and found some activation in the FFA when car experts were identifying cars and when bird experts were identifying birds.<ref name="Gauthier">{{cite journal|author-link=Isabel Gauthier|vauthors=Gauthier I, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Anderson AW|date=Feb 2000|title=Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition|journal=Nat. Neurosci.|volume=3|issue=2|pages=191–7|doi=10.1038/72140|pmid=10649576|s2cid=15752722}}<!--| access-date= 2007-12-04 --></ref> This finding has been replicated,<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Xu|first=Y.|date=2005-08-01|title=Revisiting the Role of the Fusiform Face Area in Visual Expertise|journal=Cerebral Cortex|language=en|volume=15|issue=8|pages=1234–1242|doi=10.1093/cercor/bhi006|issn=1047-3211|pmid=15677350|doi-access=free}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last1=McGugin|first1=Rankin Williams|last2=Gatenby|first2=J. Christopher|last3=Gore|first3=John C.|last4=Gauthier|first4=Isabel|date=2012-10-16|title=High-resolution imaging of expertise reveals reliable object selectivity in the fusiform face area related to perceptual performance|journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences|language=en|volume=109|issue=42|pages=17063–17068|doi=10.1073/pnas.1116333109|issn=0027-8424|pmc=3479484|pmid=23027970|bibcode=2012PNAS..10917063M|doi-access=free}}</ref> and expertise effects in the FFA have been found for other categories such as chess displays<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Bilalić|first1=Merim|last2=Langner|first2=Robert|last3=Ulrich|first3=Rolf|last4=Grodd|first4=Wolfgang|date=2011-07-13|title=Many Faces of Expertise: Fusiform Face Area in Chess Experts and Novices|journal=Journal of Neuroscience|volume=31|issue=28|pages=10206–10214|doi=10.1523/jneurosci.5727-10.2011|pmid=21752997|pmc=6623046|doi-access=free}}</ref> and X-rays.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Bilalić|first1=Merim|last2=Grottenthaler|first2=Thomas|last3=Nägele|first3=Thomas|last4=Lindig|first4=Tobias|date=2016-03-01|title=The Faces in Radiological Images: Fusiform Face Area Supports Radiological Expertise|journal=Cerebral Cortex|language=en|volume=26|issue=3|pages=1004–1014|doi=10.1093/cercor/bhu272|pmid=25452573|issn=1047-3211|doi-access=free}}</ref> Recently, it was found that the thickness of the cortex in the FFA predicts the ability to recognize faces as well as vehicles.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=McGugin|first1=Rankin W.|last2=Van Gulick|first2=Ana E.|last3=Gauthier|first3=Isabel|date=2015-10-06|title=Cortical Thickness in Fusiform Face Area Predicts Face and Object Recognition Performance|journal=Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience|volume=28|issue=2|pages=282–294|doi=10.1162/jocn_a_00891|issn=0898-929X|pmc=5034353|pmid=26439272}}</ref>


A 2009 [[magnetoencephalography]] study found that objects incidentally perceived as faces, an example of [[pareidolia]], evoke an early (165-millisecond) activation in the FFA, at a time and location similar to that evoked by faces, whereas other common objects do not evoke such [[activation]]. This activation is similar to a face-specific ERP component [[N170]]. The authors suggest that face perception evoked by face-like objects is a relatively early process, and not a late cognitive reinterpretation phenomenon.<ref name="pmid19218867">{{cite journal |vauthors=Hadjikhani N, Kveraga K, Naik P, Ahlfors SP |title=Early (N170) activation of face-specific cortex by face-like objects |journal=[[NeuroReport]] |volume= 20|issue= 4|pages= 403–7|date=February 2009 |pmid=19218867 |doi=10.1097/WNR.0b013e328325a8e1 |url= |pmc=2713437}}</ref>
A 2009 [[magnetoencephalography]] study found that objects incidentally perceived as faces, an example of [[pareidolia]], evoke an early (165-millisecond) activation in the FFA, at a time and location similar to that evoked by faces, whereas other common objects do not evoke such [[activation]]. This activation is similar to a face-specific ERP component [[N170]]. The authors suggest that face perception evoked by face-like objects is a relatively early process, and not a late cognitive reinterpretation phenomenon.<ref name="pmid19218867">{{cite journal |vauthors=Hadjikhani N, Kveraga K, Naik P, Ahlfors SP |title=Early (N170) activation of face-specific cortex by face-like objects |journal=[[NeuroReport]] |volume= 20|issue= 4|pages= 403–7|date=February 2009 |pmid=19218867 |doi=10.1097/WNR.0b013e328325a8e1 |pmc=2713437}}</ref>


One [[case study]] of [[agnosia]] provided evidence that faces are processed in a special way. A patient known as C. K., who suffered [[brain damage]] as a result of a car accident, later developed object [[agnosia]]. He experienced great difficulty with basic-level object recognition, also extending to body parts, but performed very well at recognizing faces.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Behrmann M, Moscovitch M, Winocur G |title=Intact visual imagery and impaired visual perception in a patient with visual agnosia |journal=J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform |volume=20 |issue=5 |pages=1068–87 |date=October 1994 |pmid=7964528 |doi= 10.1037/0096-1523.20.5.1068|url=http://content.apa.org/journals/xhp/20/5/1068}}</ref> A later study showed that C. K. was unable to recognize faces that were inverted or otherwise distorted, even in cases where they could easily be identified by normal subjects.<ref>{{cite journal| title=What is special about face recognition? Nineteen experiments on a person with visual object agnosia and dyslexia but normal face recognition |vauthors=Moscovitch M, Winocur G, Behrmann M |journal = J Cogn Neurosci | volume=9 | pages=555–604| year=1997| doi=10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.555| issue=5 | pmid=23965118}}</ref> This is taken as evidence that the fusiform face area is specialized for processing faces in a normal orientation.
One [[case study]] of [[agnosia]] provided evidence that faces are processed in a special way. A patient known as C. K., who suffered [[brain damage]] as a result of a car accident, later developed object agnosia. He experienced great difficulty with basic-level object recognition, also extending to body parts, but performed very well at recognizing faces.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Behrmann M, Moscovitch M, Winocur G |title=Intact visual imagery and impaired visual perception in a patient with visual agnosia |journal=J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform |volume=20 |issue=5 |pages=1068–87 |date=October 1994 |pmid=7964528 |doi= 10.1037/0096-1523.20.5.1068|url=http://content.apa.org/journals/xhp/20/5/1068}}</ref> A later study showed that C. K. was unable to recognize faces that were inverted or otherwise distorted, even in cases where they could easily be identified by normal subjects.<ref>{{cite journal| title=What is special about face recognition? Nineteen experiments on a person with visual object agnosia and dyslexia but normal face recognition |vauthors=Moscovitch M, Winocur G, Behrmann M |journal = J Cogn Neurosci | volume=9 | pages=555–604| year=1997| doi=10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.555| issue=5 | pmid=23965118|s2cid=207550378 }}</ref> This is taken as evidence that the fusiform face area is specialized for processing faces in a normal orientation.


Studies using [[functional magnetic resonance imaging]] and [[electrocorticography]] have demonstrated that activity in the FFA codes for individual faces<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal|last=Ghuman|first=Avniel Singh|last2=Brunet|first2=Nicolas M.|last3=Li|first3=Yuanning|last4=Konecky|first4=Roma O.|last5=Pyles|first5=John A.|last6=Walls|first6=Shawn A.|last7=Destefino|first7=Vincent|last8=Wang|first8=Wei|last9=Richardson|first9=R. Mark|date=2014-01-01|title=Dynamic encoding of face information in the human fusiform gyrus|journal=Nature Communications|volume=5|pages=5672|doi=10.1038/ncomms6672|issn=2041-1723|pmc=4339092|pmid=25482825|bibcode=2014NatCo...5E5672G}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last=Anzellotti|first=Stefano|last2=Fairhall|first2=Scott L.|last3=Caramazza|first3=Alfonso|date=2014-08-01|title=Decoding representations of face identity that are tolerant to rotation|journal=Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991)|volume=24|issue=8|pages=1988–1995|doi=10.1093/cercor/bht046|issn=1460-2199|pmid=23463339}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last=Nestor|first=Adrian|last2=Plaut|first2=David C.|last3=Behrmann|first3=Marlene|date=2011-06-14|title=Unraveling the distributed neural code of facial identity through spatiotemporal pattern analysis|journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America|volume=108|issue=24|pages=9998–10003|doi=10.1073/pnas.1102433108|issn=1091-6490|pmc=3116398|pmid=21628569|bibcode=2011PNAS..108.9998N}}</ref> and the FFA is tuned for behaviorally relevant facial features.<ref name=":0" /> An [[electrocorticography]] study found that the FFA is involved in multiple stages of face processing, continuously from when people see a face until they respond to it, demonstrating the dynamic and important role the FFA plays as part of the face perception network.<ref name=":0" />
Studies using [[functional magnetic resonance imaging]] and [[electrocorticography]] have demonstrated that activity in the FFA codes for individual faces<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal|last1=Ghuman|first1=Avniel Singh|last2=Brunet|first2=Nicolas M.|last3=Li|first3=Yuanning|last4=Konecky|first4=Roma O.|last5=Pyles|first5=John A.|last6=Walls|first6=Shawn A.|last7=Destefino|first7=Vincent|last8=Wang|first8=Wei|last9=Richardson|first9=R. Mark|date=2014-01-01|title=Dynamic encoding of face information in the human fusiform gyrus|journal=Nature Communications|volume=5|page=5672|doi=10.1038/ncomms6672|issn=2041-1723|pmc=4339092|pmid=25482825|bibcode=2014NatCo...5.5672G}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Anzellotti|first1=Stefano|last2=Fairhall|first2=Scott L.|last3=Caramazza|first3=Alfonso|date=2014-08-01|title=Decoding representations of face identity that are tolerant to rotation|journal=Cerebral Cortex|volume=24|issue=8|pages=1988–1995|doi=10.1093/cercor/bht046|issn=1460-2199|pmid=23463339|doi-access=free}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Nestor|first1=Adrian|last2=Plaut|first2=David C.|last3=Behrmann|first3=Marlene|date=2011-06-14|title=Unraveling the distributed neural code of facial identity through spatiotemporal pattern analysis|journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America|volume=108|issue=24|pages=9998–10003|doi=10.1073/pnas.1102433108|issn=1091-6490|pmc=3116398|pmid=21628569|bibcode=2011PNAS..108.9998N|doi-access=free}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Khan|first1=Sheraz|last2=Gramfort|first2=Alexandre|last3=Shetty|first3=Nandita R.|last4=Kitzbichler|first4=Manfred G.|last5=Ganesan|first5=Santosh|last6=Moran|first6=Joseph M.|last7=Lee|first7=Su Mei|last8=Gabrieli|first8=John D. E.|last9=Tager-Flusberg|first9=Helen B.|date=2013-02-19|title=Local and long-range functional connectivity is reduced in concert in autism spectrum disorders|journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences|language=en|volume=110|issue=8|pages=3107–3112|doi=10.1073/pnas.1214533110|issn=0027-8424|pmid=23319621|pmc=3581984|bibcode=2013PNAS..110.3107K|doi-access=free}}</ref> and the FFA is tuned for behaviorally relevant facial features.<ref name=":0" /> An electrocorticography study found that the FFA is involved in multiple stages of face processing, continuously from when people see a face until they respond to it, demonstrating the dynamic and important role the FFA plays as part of the face perception network.<ref name=":0" />


Another study found that there is stronger activity in the FFA when a person sees a familiar face as opposed to an unfamiliar one. Participants were shown different pictures of faces that either had the same identity, familiar, or faces with separate identities, or unfamiliar. It found that participants were more accurate at matching familiar faces than unfamiliar ones. Using an fMRI, they also found that the participants that were more accurate in identifying familiar faces had more activity in their right fusiform face area and participants that were poor at matching had less activity in their right fusiform area.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Weibert|first1=K|last2=Andrews|first2=TJ|title=Activity in the right fusiform face area predicts the behavioural advantage for the perception of familiar faces.|journal=Neuropsychologia|date=August 2015|volume=75|pages=588–96|pmid=26187507|doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.07.015}}</ref>
Another study found that there is stronger activity in the FFA when a person sees a familiar face as opposed to an unfamiliar one. Participants were shown different pictures of faces that either had the same identity, familiar, or faces with separate identities, or unfamiliar. It found that participants were more accurate at matching familiar faces than unfamiliar ones. Using an fMRI, they also found that the participants that were more accurate in identifying familiar faces had more activity in their right fusiform face area and participants that were poor at matching had less activity in their right fusiform area.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Weibert|first1=K|last2=Andrews|first2=TJ|title=Activity in the right fusiform face area predicts the behavioural advantage for the perception of familiar faces.|journal=Neuropsychologia|date=August 2015|volume=75|pages=588–96|pmid=26187507|doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.07.015|s2cid=17278436}}</ref>

In 2020, scientists showed the area is also activated in people born blind.<ref name="blind_from_birth">{{Cite web|title=Face-specific brain area responds to faces even in people born blind|url=https://news.mit.edu/2020/fusiform-brain-faces-blind-0826|access-date=2021-03-06|website=MIT News {{!}} Massachusetts Institute of Technology|language=en}}</ref>


==History==
==History==
Line 29: Line 33:
The fusiform face area (FFA) is a part of the brain located in the [[fusiform gyrus]] with a debated purpose. Some researchers believe that the FFA is evolutionary purposed for [[face perception]]. Others believe that the FFA discriminates between any familiar stimuli.
The fusiform face area (FFA) is a part of the brain located in the [[fusiform gyrus]] with a debated purpose. Some researchers believe that the FFA is evolutionary purposed for [[face perception]]. Others believe that the FFA discriminates between any familiar stimuli.


Psychologists debate whether the FFA is activated by faces for an evolutionary or [[expertise]] reason. The conflicting hypotheses stem from the ambiguity in FFA activation, as the FFA is activated by both familiar objects and faces. A study regarding novel objects called [[Greeble (psychology)|greebles]] determined this phenomenon.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Gauthier|first=I|author2=Behrmann M Tarr MJ|title=Can Face Recognition Really be Dissociated from Object Recognition?|journal=Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience|year=1999|volume=11|issue=4|pages=349–70|doi=10.1162/089892999563472|pmid=10471845}}</ref> When first exposed to greebles, a person's FFA was activated more strongly by faces than by greebles. After familiarising themselves with individual greebles or becoming a greeble expert, a person's FFA was activated equally by faces and greebles. Likewise, children with [[autism]] have been shown to develop object recognition at a similarly impaired pace as face recognition.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Scherf|first=S|author2=Behrmann M |author3=Minshew N |author4=Luna B|title=Atypical Development of Face and Greeble Recognition in Autism|journal=Journal of Child Psychiatry|date=April 2008|volume=49|issue=8|pages=838–47|doi=10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01903.x}}</ref> Studies of late patients of autism have discovered that autistic people have lower neuron densities in the FFA<ref>{{cite journal|vauthors=van Kooten IA, Palmen SJ, von Cappeln P, Steinbusch HW, Korr H, Heinsen H, Hof PR, van Engeland H, Schmitz C|title=Neurons in the Fusiform Gyrus are Fewer and Smaller in Autism|journal=Brain|date=April 2008|volume=131|issue=4|pages=987–99|doi=10.1093/brain/awn033|pmid=18332073}}</ref> This raises an interesting question, however: Is the poor face perception due to a reduced number of cells or is there a reduced number of cells because autistic people seldom perceive faces?<ref>{{cite book|last=Gazzaniga|first=Michael|last2=Ivry|first2=Richard B.|last3=Mangun|first3=George R.|title=Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind|edition=4th|year=2014|publisher=W.W. Norton Company Inc.|location=New York City|isbn=978-0-393-91348-4|page=247}}</ref> Asked simply: Are faces simply objects with which every person has expertise?
Psychologists debate whether the FFA is activated by faces for an evolutionary or [[expertise]] reason. The conflicting hypotheses stem from the ambiguity in FFA activation, as the FFA is activated by both familiar objects and faces. A study regarding novel objects called [[Greeble (psychology)|greebles]] determined this phenomenon.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Gauthier|first=I|author2=Behrmann M Tarr MJ|title=Can Face Recognition Really be Dissociated from Object Recognition?|journal=Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience|year=1999|volume=11|issue=4|pages=349–70|doi=10.1162/089892999563472|pmid=10471845|citeseerx=10.1.1.34.4412|s2cid=7111762}}</ref> When first exposed to greebles, a person's FFA was activated more strongly by faces than by greebles. After familiarising themselves with individual greebles or becoming a greeble expert, a person's FFA was activated equally by faces and greebles. Likewise, children with [[autism]] have been shown to develop object recognition at a similarly impaired pace as face recognition.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Scherf|first=S|author2=Behrmann M |author3=Minshew N |author4=Luna B|title=Atypical Development of Face and Greeble Recognition in Autism|journal=Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry|date=April 2008|volume=49|issue=8|pages=838–47|doi=10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01903.x|pmid=18422548|pmc=3071970}}</ref> Studies of late patients of autism have discovered that autistic people have lower neuron densities in the FFA.<ref>{{cite journal|vauthors=van Kooten IA, Palmen SJ, von Cappeln P, Steinbusch HW, Korr H, Heinsen H, Hof PR, van Engeland H, Schmitz C|title=Neurons in the Fusiform Gyrus are Fewer and Smaller in Autism|journal=Brain|date=April 2008|volume=131|issue=4|pages=987–99|doi=10.1093/brain/awn033|pmid=18332073|doi-access=free}}</ref> This raises an interesting question, however: Is the poor face perception due to a reduced number of cells or is there a reduced number of cells because autistic people seldom perceive faces?<ref>{{cite book|last1=Gazzaniga|first1=Michael|last2=Ivry|first2=Richard B.|last3=Mangun|first3=George R.|title=Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind|url=https://archive.org/details/cognitiveneurosc0000gazz|url-access=registration|edition=4th|year=2014|publisher=W.W. Norton Company Inc.|location=New York City|isbn=978-0-393-91348-4|page=[https://archive.org/details/cognitiveneurosc0000gazz/page/247 247]}}</ref> Asked simply: Are faces simply objects with which every person has expertise?
[[File:Hanyu trad simp.svg|thumbnail|right|Chinese characters similar to those used in Fu et al., which elicit a response in the FFA.]] There is evidence supporting the FFA's evolutionary face-perception. Case studies into other dedicated areas of the brain may suggest that the FFA is intrinsically designed to recognize faces. Other studies have recognized areas of the brain essential to recognizing environments and bodies.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Epstein|first=Russell|author2=Kanwisher, Nancy|title=A cortical representation of the local visual environment|journal=Nature|date=April 1998|volume=392|issue=6676|pages=598–601|doi=10.1038/33402|pmid=9560155|bibcode=1998Natur.392..598E}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last=Downing|first=Paul|author2=Yuhong Jiang |author3=Miles Shuman |author4=Nancy Kanwisher |title=A Cortical Area Selective for Visual Processing of the Human Body|journal=Science|date=September 2001|volume=293|issue=5539|pages=2470–2473|doi=10.1126/science.1063414|pmid=11577239|bibcode=2001Sci...293.2470D}}</ref> Without these dedicated areas, people are incapable of recognizing places and bodies. Similar research regarding [[prosopagnosia]] has determined that the FFA is essential to the recognition of unique faces.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Liu|first=J|author2=Kanwisher N Harris A|title=Perception of Face Parts and Face Configuration: an fMRI Study|journal=Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience|year=2010|volume=22|issue=1|pages=203–11|doi=10.1162/jocn.2009.21203|pmid=19302006|pmc=2888696}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last=Prieto|first=EA|author2=Caharel S |author3=Henson R |author4=Rossion B|title=Face-sensitivity Despite Right Lateral Occipital Brain Damage in Acquired Prosopagnosia|journal=Frontiers in Human Neuroscience|year=2011|volume=5|page=138|doi=10.3389/fnhum.2011.00138}}</ref> However, these patients are capable of recognizing the same people normally by other means, such as voice. Studies involving language characters have also been conducted in order to ascertain the role of the FFA in face recognition. These studies have found that objects, such as [[Chinese character|Chinese]] characters, elicit a high response in different areas of the FFA than those areas that elicit a high response from faces.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Fu|first=S|author2=Chunliang F |author3=Shichun G |author4=Yuejia L |author5=Raja P|title=Neural Adaptation Provides Evidence for Categorical Differences in Processing of Faces and Chinese Characters: an ERP Study of the N170|journal=PLoS ONE|year=2012|volume=7|issue=7|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0041103|editor1-last=Barton|editor1-first=Jason Jeremy Sinclair|pages=e41103|pmid=22911750|pmc=3404057|bibcode=2012PLoSO...741103F}}</ref> This data implies that certain areas of the FFA have evolutionary face-perception purposes.
[[File:Hanyu trad simp.svg|thumbnail|right|Chinese characters similar to those used in Fu et al., which elicit a response in the FFA]] There is evidence supporting the FFA's evolutionary face-perception. Case studies into other dedicated areas of the brain may suggest that the FFA is intrinsically designed to recognize faces. Other studies have recognized areas of the brain essential to recognizing environments and bodies.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Epstein|first=Russell|author2=Kanwisher, Nancy|title=A cortical representation of the local visual environment|journal=Nature|date=April 1998|volume=392|issue=6676|pages=598–601|doi=10.1038/33402|pmid=9560155|bibcode=1998Natur.392..598E|s2cid=920141}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last=Downing|first=Paul|author2=Yuhong Jiang |author3=Miles Shuman |author4=Nancy Kanwisher |title=A Cortical Area Selective for Visual Processing of the Human Body|journal=Science|date=September 2001|volume=293|issue=5539|pages=2470–2473|doi=10.1126/science.1063414|pmid=11577239|bibcode=2001Sci...293.2470D|citeseerx=10.1.1.70.6526|s2cid=1564641}}</ref> Without these dedicated areas, people are incapable of recognizing places and bodies. Similar research regarding [[prosopagnosia]] has determined that the FFA is essential to the recognition of unique faces.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Liu|first=J|author2=Kanwisher N Harris A|title=Perception of Face Parts and Face Configuration: an fMRI Study|journal=Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience|year=2010|volume=22|issue=1|pages=203–11|doi=10.1162/jocn.2009.21203|pmid=19302006|pmc=2888696}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last=Prieto|first=EA|author2=Caharel S |author3=Henson R |author4=Rossion B|title=Face-sensitivity Despite Right Lateral Occipital Brain Damage in Acquired Prosopagnosia|journal=Frontiers in Human Neuroscience|year=2011|volume=5|page=138|doi=10.3389/fnhum.2011.00138|pmid=22275889|pmc=3257870|doi-access=free}}</ref> However, these patients are capable of recognizing the same people normally by other means, such as voice. Studies involving language characters have also been conducted in order to ascertain the role of the FFA in face recognition. These studies have found that objects, such as [[Chinese character|Chinese]] characters, elicit a high response in different areas of the FFA than those areas that elicit a high response from faces.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Fu|first=S|author2=Chunliang F |author3=Shichun G |author4=Yuejia L |author5=Raja P|title=Neural Adaptation Provides Evidence for Categorical Differences in Processing of Faces and Chinese Characters: an ERP Study of the N170|journal=PLOS ONE|year=2012|volume=7|issue=7|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0041103|editor1-last=Barton|editor1-first=Jason Jeremy Sinclair|pages=e41103|pmid=22911750|pmc=3404057|bibcode=2012PLoSO...741103F|doi-access=free}}</ref> This data implies that certain areas of the FFA have evolutionary face-perception purposes.


===Evidence from infants===
===Evidence from infants===
The FFA is underdeveloped in children and does not fully develop until adolescence. This calls into question the evolutionary purpose of the FFA, as children show the ability to differentiate faces. Two-year-old babies have been shown to prefer the face of their mother.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Bushnell|first=I.W.R.|title=Mother's Face Recognition in Newborn Infants: Learning and Memory|journal=Infant and Child Development|year=2001|volume=10|pages=67–74|doi=10.1002/icd.248}}</ref> Although the FFA is underdeveloped in two-year-old babies, they have the ability to recognize their mother. Babies as early as three months old have shown the ability to distinguish between faces.<ref>{{cite book|last=Goldstein|first=Bruce|title=Sense and Perception|year=2013|publisher=Cengage Lerning|location=Belmont, CA|isbn=978-1133958499|page=91}}</ref> During this time, babies exhibit the ability to differentiate between genders, showing a clear preference for female faces.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Quinn|first=P.C.|author2=Yahr J|author3= Kuhn A.|author4= Slater A.M. |author5=Pascalils O.|title=Representation of the Gender of Human Faces by Infants: a Preference for Female|journal=Perception|year=2002|volume=31|issue=9|pages=1109–21|pmid=12375875|doi=10.1068/p3331}}</ref> It is theorized that, in terms of evolution, babies focus on women for food, although the preference could simply reflect a bias for the caregivers they experience. Infants do not appear to use this area for the perception of faces. Recent fMRI work has found no face selective area in the brain of infants 4 to 6 months old.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Deen|first=Ben|last2=Richardson|first2=Hilary|last3=Dilks|first3=Daniel D.|last4=Takahashi|first4=Atsushi|last5=Keil|first5=Boris|last6=Wald|first6=Lawrence L.|last7=Kanwisher|first7=Nancy|last8=Saxe|first8=Rebecca|date=2017-01-10|title=Organization of high-level visual cortex in human infants|url=http://www.nature.com/doifinder/10.1038/ncomms13995|journal=Nature Communications|language=en|volume=8|pages=13995|doi=10.1038/ncomms13995|issn=2041-1723|pmc=5234071|pmid=28072399|bibcode=2017NatCo...813995D}}</ref> However, given that the adult human brain has been studied far more extensively than the infant brain, and that infants are still undergoing major neurodevelopmental processes, it may simply be that the FFA is not located in anatomically familiar area. It may also be that activation for many different percepts and cognitive tasks in infants is diffuse in terms of neural circuitry, as infants are still undergoing periods of [[neurogenesis]] and [[neural pruning]]; this may make it more difficult to distinguish the signal, or what we would imagine as visual and complex familiar objects (like faces), from the noise, including static firing rates of neurons, and activity that is dedicated to a different task entirely than the activity of face processing. Infant vision involves only light and dark recognition, recognizing only major features of the face, activating the [[amygdala]]. These findings question the evolutionary purpose of the FFA.
The FFA is underdeveloped in children and does not fully develop until adolescence. This calls into question the evolutionary purpose of the FFA, as children show the ability to differentiate faces. Three-day-old babies have been shown to prefer the face of their mother.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Bushnell|first=I.W.R.|title=Mother's Face Recognition in Newborn Infants: Learning and Memory|journal=Infant and Child Development|year=2001|volume=10|issue=1–2|pages=67–74|doi=10.1002/icd.248|citeseerx=10.1.1.569.3165}}</ref> Babies as early as three months old have shown the ability to distinguish between faces.<ref>{{cite book|last=Goldstein|first=Bruce|title=Sense and Perception|year=2013|publisher=Cengage Lerning|location=Belmont, CA|isbn=978-1-133-95849-9|page=91}}</ref> During this time, babies may exhibit the ability to differentiate between genders, with some evidence suggesting that they prefer faces of the same sex as their primary caregiver.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Quinn|first=P.C.|author2=Yahr J|author3= Kuhn A.|author4= Slater A.M. |author5=Pascalils O.|s2cid=11359932|title=Representation of the Gender of Human Faces by Infants: a Preference for Female|journal=Perception|year=2002|volume=31|issue=9|pages=1109–21|pmid=12375875|doi=10.1068/p3331}}</ref> It is theorized that, in terms of evolution, babies focus on women for food, although the preference could simply reflect a bias for the caregivers they experience. Infants do not appear to use this area for the perception of faces. Recent fMRI work has found no face selective area in the brain of infants 4 to 6 months old.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Deen|first1=Ben|last2=Richardson|first2=Hilary|last3=Dilks|first3=Daniel D.|last4=Takahashi|first4=Atsushi|last5=Keil|first5=Boris|last6=Wald|first6=Lawrence L.|last7=Kanwisher|first7=Nancy|last8=Saxe|first8=Rebecca|date=2017-01-10|title=Organization of high-level visual cortex in human infants|journal=Nature Communications|language=en|volume=8|page=13995|doi=10.1038/ncomms13995|issn=2041-1723|pmc=5234071|pmid=28072399|bibcode=2017NatCo...813995D}}</ref> However, given that the adult human brain has been studied far more extensively than the infant brain, and that infants are still undergoing major neurodevelopmental processes, it may simply be that the FFA is not located in an anatomically familiar area. It may also be that activation for many different percepts and cognitive tasks in infants is diffuse in terms of neural circuitry, as infants are still undergoing periods of [[neurogenesis]] and [[neural pruning]]; this may make it more difficult to distinguish the signal, or what we would imagine as visual and complex familiar objects (like faces), from the noise, including static firing rates of neurons, and activity that is dedicated to a different task entirely than the activity of face processing. Infant vision involves only light and dark recognition, recognizing only major features of the face, activating the [[amygdala]]. These findings question the evolutionary purpose of the FFA.

=== Evidence from emotions ===


===Evidence from emotions===
Studies into what else may trigger the FFA validates arguments about its evolutionary purpose. There are countless [[facial expressions]] humans use that disturb the structure of the face. These disruptions and emotions are first processed in the amygdala and later transmitted to the FFA for facial recognition. This data is then used by the FFA to determine more static information about the face.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Adolphs|first=R|title=Neural Systems for Recognizing Emotion|journal=Current Opinion in Neurobiology|date=April 2002|volume=12|issue=2|pages=169–71|doi=10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00301-X}}</ref> The fact that the FFA is so far downstream in the processing of emotion suggests that it has little to do with emotion perception and instead deals in face perception.
Studies into what else may trigger the FFA validates arguments about its evolutionary purpose. There are countless [[facial expressions]] humans use that disturb the structure of the face. These disruptions and emotions are first processed in the amygdala and later transmitted to the FFA for facial recognition. This data is then used by the FFA to determine more static information about the face.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Adolphs|first=R|title=Neural Systems for Recognizing Emotion|journal=Current Opinion in Neurobiology|date=April 2002|volume=12|issue=2|pages=169–71|doi=10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00301-X|pmid=12015233|s2cid=13169882}}</ref> The fact that the FFA is so far downstream in the processing of emotion suggests that it has little to do with emotion perception and instead deals in face perception.


Recent evidence, however, shows that the FFA has other functions regarding emotion. The FFA is differentially activated by faces exhibiting different emotions. A study has determined that the FFA is activated more strongly by fearful faces than neutral faces.<ref>{{cite journal|authors=Guyer, Amanda; Monk, Christopher S. McClure-Tone, Erin B. Nelson, Eric E. Roberson-Nay, Roxann Adler, Abby D. Fromm, Stephen J. Leibenluft, Ellen Pine, Daniel S. Ernst, Monique|title=A Developmental Examination of Amygdala Response to Facial Expressions|journal=Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience|date=July 2010|volume=20|issue=9|pages=1565–82|doi=10.1162/jocn.2008.20114|pmid=18345988|pmc=2902865}}</ref> This implies that the FFA has functions in processing emotion despite its downstream processing and questions its evolutionary purpose to identify faces.
Recent evidence, however, shows that the FFA has other functions regarding emotion. The FFA is differentially activated by faces exhibiting different emotions. A study has determined that the FFA is activated more strongly by fearful faces than neutral faces.<ref>{{cite journal|vauthors=Guyer AE, Monk CS, McClure-Tone EB, Nelson EE, Roberson-Nay R, Adler AD, Fromm SJ, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, Ernst M |title=A Developmental Examination of Amygdala Response to Facial Expressions|journal=Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience|date=July 2010|volume=20|issue=9|pages=1565–82|doi=10.1162/jocn.2008.20114|pmid=18345988|pmc=2902865}}</ref> This implies that the FFA has functions in processing emotion despite its downstream processing and questions its evolutionary purpose to identify faces.


== Additional images ==
==Additional images==
<gallery>
<gallery>
File:Fusiform Face Area - animation1.gif|Fusiform face area shown in red.
File:Fusiform Face Area - animation1.gif|Fusiform face area shown in red
</gallery>
</gallery>


== See also ==
==See also==
* [[Neural processing for individual categories of objects]]
* [[Neural processing for individual categories of objects]]
* [[Super recognisers]]
* [[Super recognisers]]
* [[Cross-race effect]]


==References==
==References==
{{Reflist|3}}
{{Reflist|30em}}


==Further reading==
==Further reading==
* McKone et al., Trends in Cognitive Science, 2007
* McKone et al., Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2007
* Carlson, Neil R., Physiology of Behavior, 9th ed., 2007. {{ISBN|0-205-46724-5}}
* Carlson, Neil R., Physiology of Behavior, 9th ed., 2007. {{ISBN|0-205-46724-5}}
* {{cite journal |author1=Bukach C. M. |author2=Gauthier I. |author3=Tarr M. | year = 2006 | title = Beyond faces and modularity: The power of an expertise framework | doi = 10.1016/j.tics.2006.02.004| journal = Trends in Cognitive Sciences | volume = 10 | issue = | pages = 159–166 }}
* {{cite journal |author1=Bukach C. M. |author2=Gauthier I. |author3=Tarr M. | year = 2006 | title = Beyond faces and modularity: The power of an expertise framework | doi = 10.1016/j.tics.2006.02.004|pmid=16516534 | journal = Trends in Cognitive Sciences | volume = 10 | issue = 4| pages = 159–166 |s2cid=17207613 }}


{{Commons category|Fusiform face area}}
{{Commons category|Fusiform face area}}

Latest revision as of 19:45, 9 November 2023

Fusiform face area
Human brain, bottom view. Fusiform face area shown in bright blue.
Computer-enhanced fMRI scan of a person who has been asked to look at faces. The image shows increased blood flow in cerebral cortex that recognizes faces (FFA).
Anatomical terminology

The fusiform face area (FFA, meaning spindle-shaped face area) is a part of the human visual system (while also activated in people blind from birth[1]) that is specialized for facial recognition.[2] It is located in the inferior temporal cortex (IT), in the fusiform gyrus (Brodmann area 37).

Structure[edit]

The FFA is located in the ventral stream on the ventral surface of the temporal lobe on the lateral side of the fusiform gyrus. It is lateral to the parahippocampal place area. It displays some lateralization, usually being larger in the right hemisphere.

The FFA was discovered and continues to be investigated in humans using positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies. Usually, a participant views images of faces, objects, places, bodies, scrambled faces, scrambled objects, scrambled places, and scrambled bodies. This is called a functional localizer. Comparing the neural response between faces and scrambled faces will reveal areas that are face-responsive, while comparing cortical activation between faces and objects will reveal areas that are face-selective.

Function[edit]

The human FFA was first described by Justine Sergent in 1992[3] and later named by Nancy Kanwisher in 1997[2] who proposed that the existence of the FFA is evidence for domain specificity in the visual system. Studies have recently shown that the FFA is composed of functional clusters that are at a finer spatial scale than prior investigations have measured.[4] Electrical stimulation of these functional clusters selectively distorts face perception, which is causal support for the role of these functional clusters in perceiving the facial image.[5] While it is generally agreed that the FFA responds more to faces than to most other categories, there is debate about whether the FFA is uniquely dedicated to face processing, as proposed by Nancy Kanwisher and others, or whether it participates in the processing of other objects. The expertise hypothesis, as championed by Isabel Gauthier and others, offers an explanation for how the FFA becomes selective for faces in most people. The expertise hypothesis suggests that the FFA is a critical part of a network that is important for individuating objects that are visually similar because they share a common configuration of parts. Gauthier et al., in an adversarial collaboration with Kanwisher,[6] tested both car and bird experts, and found some activation in the FFA when car experts were identifying cars and when bird experts were identifying birds.[7] This finding has been replicated,[8][9] and expertise effects in the FFA have been found for other categories such as chess displays[10] and X-rays.[11] Recently, it was found that the thickness of the cortex in the FFA predicts the ability to recognize faces as well as vehicles.[12]

A 2009 magnetoencephalography study found that objects incidentally perceived as faces, an example of pareidolia, evoke an early (165-millisecond) activation in the FFA, at a time and location similar to that evoked by faces, whereas other common objects do not evoke such activation. This activation is similar to a face-specific ERP component N170. The authors suggest that face perception evoked by face-like objects is a relatively early process, and not a late cognitive reinterpretation phenomenon.[13]

One case study of agnosia provided evidence that faces are processed in a special way. A patient known as C. K., who suffered brain damage as a result of a car accident, later developed object agnosia. He experienced great difficulty with basic-level object recognition, also extending to body parts, but performed very well at recognizing faces.[14] A later study showed that C. K. was unable to recognize faces that were inverted or otherwise distorted, even in cases where they could easily be identified by normal subjects.[15] This is taken as evidence that the fusiform face area is specialized for processing faces in a normal orientation.

Studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging and electrocorticography have demonstrated that activity in the FFA codes for individual faces[16][17][18][19] and the FFA is tuned for behaviorally relevant facial features.[16] An electrocorticography study found that the FFA is involved in multiple stages of face processing, continuously from when people see a face until they respond to it, demonstrating the dynamic and important role the FFA plays as part of the face perception network.[16]

Another study found that there is stronger activity in the FFA when a person sees a familiar face as opposed to an unfamiliar one. Participants were shown different pictures of faces that either had the same identity, familiar, or faces with separate identities, or unfamiliar. It found that participants were more accurate at matching familiar faces than unfamiliar ones. Using an fMRI, they also found that the participants that were more accurate in identifying familiar faces had more activity in their right fusiform face area and participants that were poor at matching had less activity in their right fusiform area.[20]

In 2020, scientists showed the area is also activated in people born blind.[1]

History[edit]

Function and controversy[edit]

The fusiform face area (FFA) is a part of the brain located in the fusiform gyrus with a debated purpose. Some researchers believe that the FFA is evolutionary purposed for face perception. Others believe that the FFA discriminates between any familiar stimuli.

Psychologists debate whether the FFA is activated by faces for an evolutionary or expertise reason. The conflicting hypotheses stem from the ambiguity in FFA activation, as the FFA is activated by both familiar objects and faces. A study regarding novel objects called greebles determined this phenomenon.[21] When first exposed to greebles, a person's FFA was activated more strongly by faces than by greebles. After familiarising themselves with individual greebles or becoming a greeble expert, a person's FFA was activated equally by faces and greebles. Likewise, children with autism have been shown to develop object recognition at a similarly impaired pace as face recognition.[22] Studies of late patients of autism have discovered that autistic people have lower neuron densities in the FFA.[23] This raises an interesting question, however: Is the poor face perception due to a reduced number of cells or is there a reduced number of cells because autistic people seldom perceive faces?[24] Asked simply: Are faces simply objects with which every person has expertise?

Chinese characters similar to those used in Fu et al., which elicit a response in the FFA

There is evidence supporting the FFA's evolutionary face-perception. Case studies into other dedicated areas of the brain may suggest that the FFA is intrinsically designed to recognize faces. Other studies have recognized areas of the brain essential to recognizing environments and bodies.[25][26] Without these dedicated areas, people are incapable of recognizing places and bodies. Similar research regarding prosopagnosia has determined that the FFA is essential to the recognition of unique faces.[27][28] However, these patients are capable of recognizing the same people normally by other means, such as voice. Studies involving language characters have also been conducted in order to ascertain the role of the FFA in face recognition. These studies have found that objects, such as Chinese characters, elicit a high response in different areas of the FFA than those areas that elicit a high response from faces.[29] This data implies that certain areas of the FFA have evolutionary face-perception purposes.

Evidence from infants[edit]

The FFA is underdeveloped in children and does not fully develop until adolescence. This calls into question the evolutionary purpose of the FFA, as children show the ability to differentiate faces. Three-day-old babies have been shown to prefer the face of their mother.[30] Babies as early as three months old have shown the ability to distinguish between faces.[31] During this time, babies may exhibit the ability to differentiate between genders, with some evidence suggesting that they prefer faces of the same sex as their primary caregiver.[32] It is theorized that, in terms of evolution, babies focus on women for food, although the preference could simply reflect a bias for the caregivers they experience. Infants do not appear to use this area for the perception of faces. Recent fMRI work has found no face selective area in the brain of infants 4 to 6 months old.[33] However, given that the adult human brain has been studied far more extensively than the infant brain, and that infants are still undergoing major neurodevelopmental processes, it may simply be that the FFA is not located in an anatomically familiar area. It may also be that activation for many different percepts and cognitive tasks in infants is diffuse in terms of neural circuitry, as infants are still undergoing periods of neurogenesis and neural pruning; this may make it more difficult to distinguish the signal, or what we would imagine as visual and complex familiar objects (like faces), from the noise, including static firing rates of neurons, and activity that is dedicated to a different task entirely than the activity of face processing. Infant vision involves only light and dark recognition, recognizing only major features of the face, activating the amygdala. These findings question the evolutionary purpose of the FFA.

Evidence from emotions[edit]

Studies into what else may trigger the FFA validates arguments about its evolutionary purpose. There are countless facial expressions humans use that disturb the structure of the face. These disruptions and emotions are first processed in the amygdala and later transmitted to the FFA for facial recognition. This data is then used by the FFA to determine more static information about the face.[34] The fact that the FFA is so far downstream in the processing of emotion suggests that it has little to do with emotion perception and instead deals in face perception.

Recent evidence, however, shows that the FFA has other functions regarding emotion. The FFA is differentially activated by faces exhibiting different emotions. A study has determined that the FFA is activated more strongly by fearful faces than neutral faces.[35] This implies that the FFA has functions in processing emotion despite its downstream processing and questions its evolutionary purpose to identify faces.

Additional images[edit]

See also[edit]

References[edit]

  1. ^ a b "Face-specific brain area responds to faces even in people born blind". MIT News | Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Retrieved 2021-03-06.
  2. ^ a b Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM (Jun 1, 1997). "The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception". J. Neurosci. 17 (11): 4302–11. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997. PMC 6573547. PMID 9151747.
  3. ^ Sergent J, Ohta S, MacDonald B (Feb 1992). "Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. A positron emission tomography study". Brain. 115 (1): 15–36. doi:10.1093/brain/115.1.15. PMID 1559150.
  4. ^ Weiner, Kevin S.; Grill-Spector, Kalanit (Oct 2010). "Sparsely-distributed organization of face and limb activations in human ventral temporal cortex". NeuroImage. 52 (4): 1559–73. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.262. PMC 3122128. PMID 20457261.
  5. ^ Parvizi J, Jacques C, Foster BL, Witthoft N, Rangarajan V, Weiner KS, Grill-Spector K (Oct 2012). "Electrical stimulation of human fusiform face-selective regions distorts face perception". J Neurosci. 32 (43): 14915–20. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2609-12.2012. PMC 3517886. PMID 23100414.
  6. ^ Gauthier, Isabel (2017-02-22). "The Quest for the FFA led to the Expertise Account of its Specialization". arXiv:1702.07038 [q-bio.NC].
  7. ^ Gauthier I, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Anderson AW (Feb 2000). "Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition". Nat. Neurosci. 3 (2): 191–7. doi:10.1038/72140. PMID 10649576. S2CID 15752722.
  8. ^ Xu, Y. (2005-08-01). "Revisiting the Role of the Fusiform Face Area in Visual Expertise". Cerebral Cortex. 15 (8): 1234–1242. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi006. ISSN 1047-3211. PMID 15677350.
  9. ^ McGugin, Rankin Williams; Gatenby, J. Christopher; Gore, John C.; Gauthier, Isabel (2012-10-16). "High-resolution imaging of expertise reveals reliable object selectivity in the fusiform face area related to perceptual performance". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 109 (42): 17063–17068. Bibcode:2012PNAS..10917063M. doi:10.1073/pnas.1116333109. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 3479484. PMID 23027970.
  10. ^ Bilalić, Merim; Langner, Robert; Ulrich, Rolf; Grodd, Wolfgang (2011-07-13). "Many Faces of Expertise: Fusiform Face Area in Chess Experts and Novices". Journal of Neuroscience. 31 (28): 10206–10214. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5727-10.2011. PMC 6623046. PMID 21752997.
  11. ^ Bilalić, Merim; Grottenthaler, Thomas; Nägele, Thomas; Lindig, Tobias (2016-03-01). "The Faces in Radiological Images: Fusiform Face Area Supports Radiological Expertise". Cerebral Cortex. 26 (3): 1004–1014. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu272. ISSN 1047-3211. PMID 25452573.
  12. ^ McGugin, Rankin W.; Van Gulick, Ana E.; Gauthier, Isabel (2015-10-06). "Cortical Thickness in Fusiform Face Area Predicts Face and Object Recognition Performance". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 28 (2): 282–294. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00891. ISSN 0898-929X. PMC 5034353. PMID 26439272.
  13. ^ Hadjikhani N, Kveraga K, Naik P, Ahlfors SP (February 2009). "Early (N170) activation of face-specific cortex by face-like objects". NeuroReport. 20 (4): 403–7. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e328325a8e1. PMC 2713437. PMID 19218867.
  14. ^ Behrmann M, Moscovitch M, Winocur G (October 1994). "Intact visual imagery and impaired visual perception in a patient with visual agnosia". J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 20 (5): 1068–87. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.5.1068. PMID 7964528.
  15. ^ Moscovitch M, Winocur G, Behrmann M (1997). "What is special about face recognition? Nineteen experiments on a person with visual object agnosia and dyslexia but normal face recognition". J Cogn Neurosci. 9 (5): 555–604. doi:10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.555. PMID 23965118. S2CID 207550378.
  16. ^ a b c Ghuman, Avniel Singh; Brunet, Nicolas M.; Li, Yuanning; Konecky, Roma O.; Pyles, John A.; Walls, Shawn A.; Destefino, Vincent; Wang, Wei; Richardson, R. Mark (2014-01-01). "Dynamic encoding of face information in the human fusiform gyrus". Nature Communications. 5: 5672. Bibcode:2014NatCo...5.5672G. doi:10.1038/ncomms6672. ISSN 2041-1723. PMC 4339092. PMID 25482825.
  17. ^ Anzellotti, Stefano; Fairhall, Scott L.; Caramazza, Alfonso (2014-08-01). "Decoding representations of face identity that are tolerant to rotation". Cerebral Cortex. 24 (8): 1988–1995. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht046. ISSN 1460-2199. PMID 23463339.
  18. ^ Nestor, Adrian; Plaut, David C.; Behrmann, Marlene (2011-06-14). "Unraveling the distributed neural code of facial identity through spatiotemporal pattern analysis". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 108 (24): 9998–10003. Bibcode:2011PNAS..108.9998N. doi:10.1073/pnas.1102433108. ISSN 1091-6490. PMC 3116398. PMID 21628569.
  19. ^ Khan, Sheraz; Gramfort, Alexandre; Shetty, Nandita R.; Kitzbichler, Manfred G.; Ganesan, Santosh; Moran, Joseph M.; Lee, Su Mei; Gabrieli, John D. E.; Tager-Flusberg, Helen B. (2013-02-19). "Local and long-range functional connectivity is reduced in concert in autism spectrum disorders". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 110 (8): 3107–3112. Bibcode:2013PNAS..110.3107K. doi:10.1073/pnas.1214533110. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 3581984. PMID 23319621.
  20. ^ Weibert, K; Andrews, TJ (August 2015). "Activity in the right fusiform face area predicts the behavioural advantage for the perception of familiar faces". Neuropsychologia. 75: 588–96. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.07.015. PMID 26187507. S2CID 17278436.
  21. ^ Gauthier, I; Behrmann M Tarr MJ (1999). "Can Face Recognition Really be Dissociated from Object Recognition?". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 11 (4): 349–70. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.34.4412. doi:10.1162/089892999563472. PMID 10471845. S2CID 7111762.
  22. ^ Scherf, S; Behrmann M; Minshew N; Luna B (April 2008). "Atypical Development of Face and Greeble Recognition in Autism". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 49 (8): 838–47. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01903.x. PMC 3071970. PMID 18422548.
  23. ^ van Kooten IA, Palmen SJ, von Cappeln P, Steinbusch HW, Korr H, Heinsen H, Hof PR, van Engeland H, Schmitz C (April 2008). "Neurons in the Fusiform Gyrus are Fewer and Smaller in Autism". Brain. 131 (4): 987–99. doi:10.1093/brain/awn033. PMID 18332073.
  24. ^ Gazzaniga, Michael; Ivry, Richard B.; Mangun, George R. (2014). Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind (4th ed.). New York City: W.W. Norton Company Inc. p. 247. ISBN 978-0-393-91348-4.
  25. ^ Epstein, Russell; Kanwisher, Nancy (April 1998). "A cortical representation of the local visual environment". Nature. 392 (6676): 598–601. Bibcode:1998Natur.392..598E. doi:10.1038/33402. PMID 9560155. S2CID 920141.
  26. ^ Downing, Paul; Yuhong Jiang; Miles Shuman; Nancy Kanwisher (September 2001). "A Cortical Area Selective for Visual Processing of the Human Body". Science. 293 (5539): 2470–2473. Bibcode:2001Sci...293.2470D. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.70.6526. doi:10.1126/science.1063414. PMID 11577239. S2CID 1564641.
  27. ^ Liu, J; Kanwisher N Harris A (2010). "Perception of Face Parts and Face Configuration: an fMRI Study". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 22 (1): 203–11. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21203. PMC 2888696. PMID 19302006.
  28. ^ Prieto, EA; Caharel S; Henson R; Rossion B (2011). "Face-sensitivity Despite Right Lateral Occipital Brain Damage in Acquired Prosopagnosia". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 5: 138. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2011.00138. PMC 3257870. PMID 22275889.
  29. ^ Fu, S; Chunliang F; Shichun G; Yuejia L; Raja P (2012). Barton, Jason Jeremy Sinclair (ed.). "Neural Adaptation Provides Evidence for Categorical Differences in Processing of Faces and Chinese Characters: an ERP Study of the N170". PLOS ONE. 7 (7): e41103. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...741103F. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0041103. PMC 3404057. PMID 22911750.
  30. ^ Bushnell, I.W.R. (2001). "Mother's Face Recognition in Newborn Infants: Learning and Memory". Infant and Child Development. 10 (1–2): 67–74. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.569.3165. doi:10.1002/icd.248.
  31. ^ Goldstein, Bruce (2013). Sense and Perception. Belmont, CA: Cengage Lerning. p. 91. ISBN 978-1-133-95849-9.
  32. ^ Quinn, P.C.; Yahr J; Kuhn A.; Slater A.M.; Pascalils O. (2002). "Representation of the Gender of Human Faces by Infants: a Preference for Female". Perception. 31 (9): 1109–21. doi:10.1068/p3331. PMID 12375875. S2CID 11359932.
  33. ^ Deen, Ben; Richardson, Hilary; Dilks, Daniel D.; Takahashi, Atsushi; Keil, Boris; Wald, Lawrence L.; Kanwisher, Nancy; Saxe, Rebecca (2017-01-10). "Organization of high-level visual cortex in human infants". Nature Communications. 8: 13995. Bibcode:2017NatCo...813995D. doi:10.1038/ncomms13995. ISSN 2041-1723. PMC 5234071. PMID 28072399.
  34. ^ Adolphs, R (April 2002). "Neural Systems for Recognizing Emotion". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 12 (2): 169–71. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00301-X. PMID 12015233. S2CID 13169882.
  35. ^ Guyer AE, Monk CS, McClure-Tone EB, Nelson EE, Roberson-Nay R, Adler AD, Fromm SJ, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, Ernst M (July 2010). "A Developmental Examination of Amygdala Response to Facial Expressions". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 20 (9): 1565–82. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20114. PMC 2902865. PMID 18345988.

Further reading[edit]